
1782 posts
A Shoe Made Of Hydrophobic Material! I Tried Posting An Awesome Gif A Few Days Ago Of Something Else

A shoe made of Hydrophobic Material! I tried posting an awesome gif a few days ago of something else to do with hydrophobic material, but for some reason it didn’t work, so this will have to do. Sorry :(
But anyway - The molecules that coat this shoe are Hydrophobic, that means that they basically ‘repel’ water molecules, while we are only just mastering this, nature developed it billions of years ago - Our cell walls are also made of molecules with similar properties!
-
witchfindertable liked this · 4 years ago
-
kimthekittycat liked this · 5 years ago
-
people-suck-give-me-cute-things liked this · 6 years ago
-
peter-nureyev-kinnie liked this · 6 years ago
-
emilooharris liked this · 7 years ago
-
thousandeyesand-one liked this · 7 years ago
-
ncc-1701cmo liked this · 7 years ago
-
lalo4tizkr liked this · 7 years ago
-
machiavelique liked this · 7 years ago
-
peachyfields1998 reblogged this · 8 years ago
-
the-jester-block liked this · 8 years ago
-
xoxososo liked this · 8 years ago
-
pint-sized-queen liked this · 8 years ago
-
rachaeljurassic reblogged this · 9 years ago
-
mayastormborn reblogged this · 9 years ago
-
floridianqueen liked this · 9 years ago
-
spacegoddessofroachkingdom liked this · 9 years ago
-
schmidts330 liked this · 9 years ago
-
thethreadsmiths reblogged this · 9 years ago
-
lifeswhtumakeit reblogged this · 9 years ago
-
color-of-my-brain reblogged this · 9 years ago
-
color-of-my-brain liked this · 9 years ago
-
whatfi reblogged this · 9 years ago
-
whatfi liked this · 9 years ago
-
cool-usr-name liked this · 9 years ago
-
rad-radley-blog reblogged this · 9 years ago
-
rad-radley-blog liked this · 9 years ago
-
wormsinyourbrain liked this · 9 years ago
-
gif-dealer liked this · 9 years ago
-
starhasarrived liked this · 9 years ago
-
mytypeofwoman87 reblogged this · 9 years ago
-
zerozeroandzero liked this · 9 years ago
-
beaniexd liked this · 9 years ago
-
alonewithyoursoul liked this · 9 years ago
-
xspacemandrewx reblogged this · 9 years ago
-
blvckdynamite reblogged this · 9 years ago
-
kiddforever liked this · 9 years ago
-
eternalgalactic liked this · 9 years ago
-
mjayvevo liked this · 9 years ago
-
mtmeru-archive liked this · 9 years ago
-
magnanimous-ant reblogged this · 9 years ago
More Posts from Themanfromnantucket
An alternative definition for the word, submitted by Tom Witte to The Washington Post's neologism contest:
Sarchasm [sahr-kaz-uhm] n: The gulf between the author of sarcastic wit and the person who doesn't get it.

To say, “This is my uncle,” in Chinese, you have no choice but to encode more information about said uncle. The language requires that you denote the side the uncle is on, whether he’s related by marriage or birth and, if it’s your father’s brother, whether he’s older or younger.
“All of this information is obligatory. Chinese doesn’t let me ignore it,” says Chen. “In fact, if I want to speak correctly, Chinese forces me to constantly think about it.”
This got Chen wondering: Is there a connection between language and how we think and behave? In particular, Chen wanted to know: does our language affect our economic decisions?
Chen designed a study — which he describes in detail in this blog post — to look at how language might affect individual’s ability to save for the future. According to his results, it does — big time.
While “futured languages,” like English, distinguish between the past, present and future, “futureless languages,” like Chinese, use the same phrasing to describe the events of yesterday, today and tomorrow. Using vast inventories of data and meticulous analysis, Chen found that huge economic differences accompany this linguistic discrepancy. Futureless language speakers are 30 percent more likely to report having saved in any given year than futured language speakers. (This amounts to 25 percent more savings by retirement, if income is held constant.) Chen’s explanation: When we speak about the future as more distinct from the present, it feels more distant — and we’re less motivated to save money now in favor of monetary comfort years down the line.
But that’s only the beginning. There’s a wide field of research on the link between language and both psychology and behavior. Here, a few fascinating examples:
Navigation and Pormpuraawans In Pormpuraaw, an Australian Aboriginal community, you wouldn’t refer to an object as on your “left” or “right,” but rather as “northeast” or “southwest,” writes Stanford psychology professor Lera Boroditsky (and an expert in linguistic-cultural connections) in the Wall Street Journal. About a third of the world’s languages discuss space in these kinds of absolute terms rather than the relative ones we use in English, according to Boroditsky. “As a result of this constant linguistic training,” she writes, “speakers of such languages are remarkably good at staying oriented and keeping track of where they are, even in unfamiliar landscapes.” On a research trip to Australia, Boroditsky and her colleague found that Pormpuraawans, who speak Kuuk Thaayorre, not only knew instinctively in which direction they were facing, but also always arranged pictures in a temporal progression from east to west.
Blame and English Speakers In the same article, Boroditsky notes that in English, we’ll often say that someone broke a vase even if it was an accident, but Spanish and Japanese speakers tend to say that the vase broke itself. Boroditsky describes a study by her student Caitlin Fausey in which English speakers were much more likely to remember who accidentally popped balloons, broke eggs, or spilled drinks in a video than Spanish or Japanese speakers. (Guilt alert!) Not only that, but there’s a correlation between a focus on agents in English and our criminal-justice bent toward punishing transgressors rather than restituting victims, Boroditsky argues.
Color among Zuñi and Russian Speakers Our ability to distinguish between colors follows the terms in which we describe them, as Chen notes in the academic paper in which he presents his research (forthcoming in the American Economic Review; PDF here). A 1954 study found that Zuñi speakers, who don’t differentiate between orange and yellow, have trouble telling them apart. Russian speakers, on the other hand, have separate words for light blue (goluboy) and dark blue (siniy). According to a 2007 study, they’re better than English speakers at picking out blues close to the goluboy/siniy threshold.
Gender in Finnish and Hebrew In Hebrew, gender markers are all over the place, whereas Finnish doesn’t mark gender at all, Boroditsky writes in Scientific American (PDF). A study done in the 1980s found that, yup, thought follows suit: kids who spoke Hebrew knew their own genders a year earlier than those who grew up speaking Finnish. (Speakers of English, in which gender referents fall in the middle, were in between on that timeline, too.)
six word poem 3/17
Prompt: “Can you please write a poem about getting lost in a book? (Thanks!)”
Holy shit, that’s a big book.